Cognitive Psychology. A student\'s handbook. Sixth Edition. (M. Eysenck und M. Keane). Kapitel6 PDF

Title Cognitive Psychology. A student\'s handbook. Sixth Edition. (M. Eysenck und M. Keane). Kapitel6
Author Le Ki
Course Kognition, Bildung, Entwicklung
Institution Universität Kassel
Pages 11
File Size 272.4 KB
File Type PDF
Total Downloads 42
Total Views 116

Summary

Download Cognitive Psychology. A student's handbook. Sixth Edition. (M. Eysenck und M. Keane). Kapitel6 PDF


Description

Chapter 6: Learning, memory and forgetting Theories of memory generally consider both the architecture of the memory system and the processes operating within that structure. Architecture refers to the way in which the memory system is organised, and process refers to the activities occurring within the memory system. Architecture or structure and process are both important, but some theorists emphasise only one in their theoretical formulations. Learning and memory involve a series of stages. Processes occurring during the presentation of the learning material are known as “encoding”. This is the first stage. As a result of encoding, some information is stored within the memory system. Thus, storage is the second stage. The third, and final, stage is retrieval, which involves recovering or extracting stored information from the memory system. We have emphasised the distinctions between architecture and process and among encoding, storage and retrieval. However, we cannot have architecture without process, or retrieval without previous encoding and storage. It is only when processes operate on the essentially passive structures of the memory system that it becomes active and of use. x

Architecture of memory

The multi-store model is based on common features of different theories. Three types of memory store were proposed: 1. Sensory store: modality specific, brief Most information in the environment is not attended to. The iconic store (or visual store) has limited capacity and duration. Sperling’s (1960) study found that information decayed in about 0.5 seconds. The echoic store is a transient auditory store holding relatively unprocessed input. Ioannides et al. (2003) found evidence for the echoic memory lasting 5 seconds in the left hemisphere and 2 seconds in the right hemisphere. 2.

Short-term store: limited capacity and fragile storage Short-term memory is very limited; for example, digit and letter spans typically have a value of 7 plus or minus 2. Miller argued that the capacity of short-term memory should be assessed by number of chunks – integrated pieces of units of information. Span is less for larger chunks. Information is lost from short-term memory through decay and interference. Berman et al. (2009) claim that new information enters short-term memory and disrupts the information already there. INTERACTIVE EXERCISE: Capacity of short-term memory INTERACTIVE EXERCISE: Duration of short-term memory

3.

Long-term store: unlimited capacity over long periods Double dissociation indicates that two tasks probably involve different processing mechanisms and provides strong evidence for the distinction between short-term and long-term memory store. Patients with amnesia have impairments of long-term memory but typically have no problem with short-term memory. They typically have damage to the medial temporal lobe. Patients may also have impaired short-term memory but intact long-term memory (see, e.g., Shallice & Warrington, 1974). These patients typically have damage to the parietal and temporal lobes.

Some information is attended to and processed by the short-term store. Some information is transferred from short- to long-term store. Transfer to long-term storage depends on rehearsal.

While the multi-store model has proven useful in explaining memory, it is oversimplified and some of the assumptions are not correct: The short-term memory is not a gateway to long-term memory (they interact); the model does not account for implicit learning; items that are attended to can be accessed more readily than other items in short-term memory and the model exaggerates the role of rehearsal. Jonides et al. (2008) have argued that the multi-store approach should be replaced with a unitary-store model. It is proposed that amnesic patients have problems with relational memory – forming novel relations or “binding” between items and their context. This is a role played by the hippocampus. Hannula et al. (2006) showed that amnesic patients were able to form strong long-term relational memories, and this could account for their good performance on short-term memory tasks. There is evidence that activation of long-term memory plays a role in short-term memory. However, the unitary-store approach has limitations: It is oversimplified in assuming that short-term memory is only activated long-term memory. Hippocampal involvement is greater in long-term memory than short-term memory and neuroimaging studies do not support the unitary-store model. According to the multi-store theory, there are separate sensory, short-term and long-term stores. There is strong evidence to support the notion of various qualitatively different memory stores, but this approach provides a very oversimplified view. For example, multi-store theorists assumed that there are unitary short-term and long-term stores, but the reality is more complex. Alternatively, unitary-store models assume that short-term memory consists of temporary activations of long-term memory. The difficulties that amnesic patients have are explained by a problem with forming novel relations. There is some evidence for unitary-store models, but these are still oversimplifications of the structure of memory. WEBLINK: An article on techniques for memory retention x

Working memory

Baddeley and Hitch (1974) and Baddeley (1986) replaced the concept of the short-term store with that of working memory. Baddeley (2001) described four components of working memory: x Central executive: o modality-free; o limited capacity; o resembles attention; o deals with cognitively demanding tasks. x Phonological loop: o holds information in speech-based form; o limited capacity. x Visuo-spatial sketchpad: o storage and manipulation of spatial and visual information; o limited capacity. x Episodic buffer Assumptions from this theory: x If two tasks use the same component, they cannot be performed successfully together. x If two tasks use separate components, it should be possible to perform them as well together as separately.

In a series of dual-task studies, Robbins et al. (1996) showed that selecting chess moves involved the central executive and visuo-spatial sketchpad but not the phonological loop. Phonological loop The phonological similarity effect is good evidence for the role of the phonological loop in short-term memory tasks. Larsen et al. (2000) found that serial recall was 25% worse if a list of words was phonologically similar. RESEARCH ACTIVITY: Phonemic similarity INTERACTIVE EXERCISE: Encoding in short-term memory The word-length effect (Baddeley et al., 1975) is the finding that memory span is better for shorter words than for longer words, which have a longer articulatory duration.This suggests that capacity of the phonological loop is determined by temporal duration. According to Baddeley (1986, 1990), the phonological loop consists of: x a passive phonological store concerned with speech perception; x an articulatory process linked to speech production. Baddeley suggests auditory presentation of words has direct access to the phonological store, but visual presentation only has indirect access via subvocal articulation. There is support from research with patients with brain damage. Patients with damage to left inferior parietal cortex have a damaged phonological store but intact articulatory control. Patients with damage to left inferior frontal cortex have intact phonological store but damaged articulatory control. Baddeley et al. (1998) suggest that the phonological loop may be more important in learning new words than familiar ones. The capacity of the phonological loop predicts vocabulary size. Visuo-spatial sketchpad The visuo-spatial sketchpad is used in the temporary storage and manipulation of spatial and visual information. Logie (1995) argues for two components of visuo-spatial working memory: x The visual cache o stores form and colour information. x The inner scribe o processes spatial and movement information; o rehearses and transfers information from the visual cache to the central executive. There is experimental support for the notion of separate visual and spatial systems. Klauer and Zhao (2004) found a spatial interference task disrupted performance more on a spatial main task than a visual main task. Smith and Jonides (1997) found differences in brain activation during different visual and spatial tasks. The ventral prefrontal cortex is more activated during visual working-memory tasks and the dorsal prefrontal cortex is more activated during spatial working-memory tasks. Many tasks require both components to be used in combination. It is unclear how information from both components is combined and integrated. Central executive The central executive is the most important and versatile component of working memory. It is thought to involve the prefrontal cortex. rTMS to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex disrupts performance on many complex cognitive tasks. Individuals with dysexecutive syndrome have problems with planning, organising, monitoring and initiating behaviour. They typically have damage within the frontal lobes.

The conceptualisation of the central executive has changed over time. Originally, the central executive was viewed as being unitary. More recent proposals highlight several executive processes such as: x Baddeley (1996): o Switching of retrieval plans. o Timesharing in dual-task studies. o Selective attention. o Temporary activation of long-term memory. x Miyake et al. (2000): o Inhibition function – the ability to deliberately inhibit dominant, automatic or prepotent responses when necessary, for example in the Stroop effect. o Shifting function – shifting back and forth between multiple tasks, operations or mental sets. o Updating function – updating and monitoring of working memory representations. INTERACTIVE EXERCISE: Stroop – automatic processing Latent variable analysis on various executive function tasks identifies three separable functions (Miyake et al., 2000). Each executive process activates a different region in the prefrontal cortex. However, all tasks activate the intraparietal sulcus. Executive processes are needed in dual-task situations. Patients with Alzheimer’s disease are more sensitive than controls to dual-task demands, but cope equally well with single tasks. This suggests Alzheimer’s patients have damage to executive processes involved in dual-task processing. Based on studies with frontal lobe-damaged patients, Stuss and Alexander (2007) disagreed with the notion of a single dysexecutive syndrome arising from frontal lobe damage. Instead, they proposed three executive processes associated with different parts of the frontal lobe: x Task setting (left lateral frontal): setting a stimulus–response relationship. x Monitoring (right lateral frontal): checking the task and behavioural adjustment. x Energisation (superior medial frontal): initiation and sustaining of a response. These processes are not independent and are used across a wide range of tasks. The central executive consists of various related but separable executive processes. Accumulating evidence points to inhibition, updating, shifting and dual-task coordination as four major executive processes. The notion of a dysexecutive syndrome is misleading as it suggests a single pattern of impairment. Further research is needed to clarify differences in the executive processes suggested by imaging on healthy individuals and those with frontal lobe damage. Episodic buffer Main characteristics (Baddeley, 2000): x Integrates and stores information from various sources into coherent complex structures or episodes. x Acts as an intermediary between the subsystems, combining them into multi-dimensional representations. x Has close links to the central executive. The episodic buffer was proposed because components in the original model were too separate in their functioning and did not allow for combination and integration of information (e.g., Chincotta et al., 1999).

WEBLINK: Iowa Gambling Task demonstration CASE STUDY: The episodic buffer Baddeley and Wilson (2002) argued that high levels of immediate prose recall depends on the capacity of the episodic buffer and its efficiency. As predicted by this model, patients with impaired long-term memory still have good immediate prose recall, and this recall is better if the amnesic patient has good executive functioning, compared to those with more severe executive deficits. The episodic buffer is a valuable addition to the working memory model and accounts for our ability to integrate different kinds of information in working memory. Some progress has been made to identifying brain areas associated with the episodic buffer, for example the hippocampus. The use of the episodic buffer is sometimes associated with the central executive. However, it is difficult to design tasks to distinguish clearly between the episodic buffer and other components. INTERACTIVE EXERCISE: Working memory There are several advantages of the working memory system over Atkinson and Shiffrin’s short-term store. Working memory: x involves both active processing and transient storage of information; x explains the partial short-term memory deficits sometimes caused by brain damage; x incorporates verbal rehearsal as an optional process. However: x It is difficult to identify the number and nature of executive processes associated with the central executive. x The understanding of the main executive processes associated with the central executive is not clear. x More research is needed on the interactions between the four components of the working memory system. Baddeley replaced the unitary short-term store with a working memory system consisting of three components: an attention-like central executive, a phonological loop holding speech-based information and a visuo-spatial sketchpad specialised for spatial and visual coding. Later, a fourth component – the episodic buffer – was added, allowing information from various sources to be integrated and combined. Some progress has been made in identifying the brain regions involved in various components of the working memory system, particularly from studies with brain-damaged patients. Although originally proposed to be unitary, the central executive is now thought to involve various separable executive processes such as inhibition, updating, shifting, updating and dual-task coordination. It is also thought that the episodic buffer is often used in conjunction with the central executive. However, it remains a challenge to identify the number and nature of executive processes, and to isolate their contributions experimentally. x

Working memory capacity

Working memory capacity refers to how much information an individual can process and store at the same time. Working memory capacity correlates with fluid intelligence (Unsworth, 2010). Participants with high working memory capacity have superior executive and/or attentional processes. This is because there are

greater individual differences with these functions than those of the visuo-spatial sketchpad and the phonological loop. It has been measured using: x the reading span task; WEBLINK: Reading span task x

the operation span. WEBLINK: Video of the operation span task

Engle and Kane (2004) indicate that there are two factors involved in working memory: the maintenance of task goals and the resolution of response conflict. Working memory capacity correlates with brain activity, such that, under distraction, those with high working memory capacity show reduced ERP responses than those with low working memory capacity (Yurgil & Golob, 2013). This work suggests working memory capacity is related to fluid intelligence and the ability to respond to novel stimuli and situations. x

Levels of processing

Craik and Lockhart (1972) proposed a framework detailing various levels of stimulus processing from shallow or physical analysis to deep or semantic analysis. The key theoretical assumptions were: x The level or depth of processing of a stimulus has a large effect on its memorability. x Deeper analysis produces more elaborate, longer-lasting and stronger memory traces. They argued that maintenance rehearsal, simply repeating previous analyses, does not enhance long-term memory. Many studies support the main assumptions of the levels-of-processing approach. Craik and Tulving (1975) showed depth of processing had major effects on memory performance – performance was three times better with deep than shallow processing. There is more brain activation during semantic than perceptual processing. Craik and Tulving (1975) argued that elaboration (the amount of processing of a particular kind) is important. Eysenck and Eysenck (1980) tested this and found words in a non-semantic, distinctive condition were better recognised than non-semantic, non-distinctive words, and almost as well as semantic words. Level-of-processing effects are greater in explicit memory tasks than in implicit memory tasks (Challis et al., 1996). Craik and Lockhart (1972) argued correctly that processes during learning have a major impact on subsequent long-term memory. Learning and remembering are by-products of perception, attention and comprehension. This approach has led to the identification of elaboration and distinctiveness as important factors in learning and memory. However, the approach possesses several limitations: the importance of the retrieval environment on performance was underestimated; the relative importance of depth, elaboration and distinctiveness of processing is unclear; many of the terms have not been defined with any precision; and it does not explain why deep processing is so effective. INTERACTIVE EXERCISE: Levels of processing Craik and Lockhart (1972) focused on learning processes in their levels-of-processing theory. They (and their followers) identified depth of processing (i.e., the extent to which meaning is processed), elaboration of processing and distinctiveness of processing as key determinants of long-term memory. Insufficient

attention was paid to the relationship between the processes at learning and those at the time of test. Other problems are that the theory is not explanatory, that it is hard to assess the depth of processing and that shallow processing can lead to very good long-term memory. x

Learning through retrieval

Practice in retrieving to-be-remembered information during the learning period can enhance long-term memory more than simply engaging in study and restudy. This strong effect is known as the testing effect (Karpicke, 2012). Dunlosky et al. (2013) have discussed ten techniques of the use of restructuring information in order to improve learning. Pyc and Rawson (2010, 2012) have suggested that, during learning through testing, people find mediators or links between the to-be-remembered items. However, Peterson and Mulligan (2013) have demonstrated that learning through testing can limit learning by preventing people from detecting underlying structures. Research on this topic has been primarily conducted through paired-associate learning and therefore may not generalise to other forms of learning. The factors that underlie mediator effectiveness are not well understood. The theory also doesn’t predict results indicating that testing can be less effective than restudying. WEBLINK: Learning through testing The studies here show that, in order to improve learning, it can be effective to test yourself during the learning process. x

Implicit learning

Implicit learning is learning without conscious awareness of having learned – the process through which we become sensitive to certain regularities in the environment (Cleeremans & Jiménez, 2002): x in the absence of intention to learn about these regularities; x in the absence of awareness that one is learning; x in such a way that the resulting knowledge is difficult to express. There are similarities between implicit learning and implicit memory but also differences pertaining to the type of knowledge acquired and stimuli used in measurement. Reber (1993) propos...


Similar Free PDFs